Fake news hurts health, especially during outbreaks like Covid-19


Misinformation, which can spread quickly online, could influence people to behave more riskily during an outbreak. — 123rf.com

The rise of fake news could be making disease outbreaks worse, according to new research from the University of East Anglia (UEA) in the United Kingdom.

The researchers focused on influenza, monkeypox and norovirus across two studies, but they say their findings could also be useful in dealing with the Covid-19 outbreak.

The team say that efforts to stop people sharing fake news, misinformation and harmful advice on social media could save lives.

The worry that fake news might be used to distort political processes or manipulate financial markets is well established.

But less studied is the possibility that misinformation spread could harm human health, especially during the outbreak of an infectious disease.

Covid-19 expert Professor Paul Hunter and Dr Julii Brainard, both from UEA’s Norwich Medical School, set out to test the effect of sharing dangerously wrong information on human health during a disease outbreak.

Prof Hunter said: “Fake news is manufactured with no respect for accuracy, and is often based on conspiracy theories.

“Worryingly, research has shown that nearly 40% of the British public believe at least one conspiracy theory, and even more in the United States and other countries.

“When it comes to Covid-19, there has been a lot of speculation, misinformation and fake news circulating on the internet – about how the virus originated, what causes it and how it is spread.

“Misinformation means that bad advice can circulate very quickly – and it can change human behaviour to take greater risks.

“We have already seen how the rise of the anti-vax (vaccine) movement has created a surge in measles cases around the world.

“People in West Africa affected by the Ebola outbreak were more likely to practise unsafe burial practices if they believed misinformation.

“And here in the UK, 14% of parents have reported sending their child to school with symptoms of contagious chickenpox – violating school policies and official quarantine advice.

“Examples of risky behaviour during infectious disease outbreaks include not washing hands, sharing food with ill people, not disinfecting potentially contaminated surfaces and failing to self-isolate.

“Worryingly, people are more likely to share bad advice on social media, than good advice from trusted sources such as the (UK) National Health Service, Public Health England or the World Health Organization (WHO).”

The researchers created theoretical simulations that took into account studies of real behaviour; how different diseases are spread, incubation periods and recovery times; the speed and frequency of social media posting; and real-life information-sharing.

They also took into account how a distrust in conventional authorities is closely linked to the tendency to believe in conspiracy theories, the phenomena that people interact within “information bubbles” online, and the fact that people are more likely to share false stories than correct information online.

The researchers also investigated strategies to fight fake news, such as drowning bad information with good information and “immunising” people against bad information though better education.

Dr Brainard said: “No previous studies have looked in such detail at how the spread of misinformation affects the spread of disease.

“We found that misinformation during epidemics of infectious disease could make those outbreaks more severe. We tested strategies to reduce misinformation.

“In our first study, focusing on the flu, monkeypox and norovirus, we found that reducing the amount of harmful advice being circulated by just 10% – from 50% to 40% – mitigated the influence of bad advice on the outcomes of a disease outbreak.

“Making 20% of the population unable to share or believe harmful advice – or “immunising” them against fake news – had the same positive effect.

“Our second study, which focused on norovirus, showed that even if 90% of the advice is good, some disease will still circulate.

“We were also interested in the ‘herd immunity’ levels required to ‘immunise’ people against fake news.

“The modelling suggests that any ‘immunity’ against bad advice reduces outbreak impacts.

“But while we used very sophisticated simulation models, it is important to remember that this is not an observational study based on real behaviour.

“The efficacy of implementing such strategies to fight fake news needs to be tested in real world settings, with costs and benefits ideally compared with real world disease reduction.”

The research was partially funded by the UK National Institute for Health Research’s Health Protection Research Units in Emergency Preparedness and Response and Gastrointestinal Infections in partnership with Public Health England.

The first study was published in the journal Simulation: Transactions of the Society for Modelling and Simulation International, and the second one in the journal Revue-Depidemiologie-Et-De-Sante-Publique.

Article type: metered
User Type: anonymous web
User Status:
Campaign ID: 18
Cxense type: free
User access status: 3
   

Did you find this article insightful?

Yes
No

100% readers found this article insightful

Next In Health

Using blockchain technology for secure data exchange in healthcare
Tackling teenage misconceptions and misinformation about HIV
The Choice Of Vitamin C For Today’s Lifestyle
Avoid these 12 common skincare ingredients
What is period poverty?
He found a lump on his testicle
Why we believe conspiracy theories
Can turmeric help in breast cancer?
Helping your child maintain a healthy weight
Milk: Contains almost all the essential nutrients needed for children

Stories You'll Enjoy